0
We're unable to sign you in at this time. Please try again in a few minutes.
Retry
We were able to sign you in, but your subscription(s) could not be found. Please try again in a few minutes.
Retry
There may be a problem with your account. Please contact the AMA Service Center to resolve this issue.
Contact the AMA Service Center:
Telephone: 1 (800) 262-2350 or 1 (312) 670-7827  *   Email: subscriptions@jamanetwork.com
Error Message ......
Original Investigation |

Physical Activity and Cognition in Women With Vascular Conditions FREE

Marie-Noël Vercambre, PhD; Francine Grodstein, ScD; JoAnn E. Manson, MD, DrPH; Meir J. Stampfer, MD, DrPH; Jae H. Kang, ScD
[+] Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Foundation of Public Health, Mutuelle Generale de l’Education Nationale, Paris, France (Dr Vercambre); and Channing Laboratory and Division of Preventive Medicine, Department of Medicine, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School (Drs Vercambre, Manson, Stampfer, and Kang), and Department of Epidemiology, Harvard School of Public Health (Drs Grodstein, Manson, Stampfer, and Kang), Boston, Massachusetts.


Arch Intern Med. 2011;171(14):1244-1250. doi:10.1001/archinternmed.2011.282.
Text Size: A A A
Published online

Background Individuals with vascular disease or risk factors have substantially higher rates of cognitive decline, yet little is known about means of maintaining cognition in this group.

Methods We examined the relation between physical activity and cognitive decline in participants of the Women's Antioxidant Cardiovascular Study, a cohort of women with prevalent vascular disease or at least 3 coronary risk factors. Recreational physical activity was assessed at baseline (October 1995 through June 1996) and every 2 years thereafter. Between December 1998 and July 2000, a total of 2809 women 65 years or older underwent a cognitive battery by telephone interview, including 5 tests of global cognition, verbal memory, and category fluency. Tests were administered 3 additional times over 5.4 years. We used multivariable-adjusted general linear models for repeated measures to compare the annual rates of cognitive score changes across levels of total physical activity and energy expended in walking, as assessed at Women's Antioxidant Cardiovascular Study baseline.

Results We found a significant trend (P < .001 for trend) toward decreasing rates of cognitive decline with increasing energy expenditure. Compared with the bottom quintile of total physical activity, significant differences in rates of cognitive decline were observed from the fourth quintile (P = .04 for the fourth quintile and P < .001 for the fifth quintile), or the equivalent of daily 30-minute walks at a brisk pace. This was equivalent to the difference in cognitive decline observed for women who were 5 to 7 years younger. Regularly walking for exercise was strongly related to slower rates of cognitive decline (P = .003 for trend).

Conclusion Regular physical activity, including walking, was associated with better preservation of cognitive function in older women with vascular disease or risk factors.

Figures in this Article

Older individuals with cardiovascular disease (CVD) or coronary risk factors have substantially increased risks of cognitive impairment. Affected are not only the domains traditionally associated with vascular health (eg, executive function) but also general cognition and episodic memory.1,2 In addition, the prevalence of CVD and risk factors has dramatically increased because of population aging and a decrease in cardiovascular mortality associated with improved treatments.3 Yet, little is known about strategies, especially behavioral approaches, to preserve cognition in this group.

Accumulating evidence suggests a protective effect of physical activity on cognition,412 but most human studies thus far have been performed among generally healthy populations. Because physical activity has been linked with reduced disease progression among individuals with CVD or vascular risk factors,13 the potential benefits of physical activity on cognition could be important in this group. Therefore, we used data from the Women's Antioxidant Cardiovascular Study to examine the relation between physical activity and cognitive decline in almost 3000 older women.

PARENT COHORT

The baseline period of the Women's Antioxidant Cardiovascular Study14 was from October 1995 through June 1996 among 8171 women as a 2 × 2 × 2 randomized placebo-controlled trial of vitamin E, vitamin C, and β-carotene supplementation for secondary prevention of CVD. Eligible participants were female health professionals at least 40 years old with 3 or more coronary risk factors (ie, diabetes mellitus, hypertension, hyperlipidemia, body mass index [calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared] ≥30, or parental history of premature myocardial infarction) or CVD (ie, stroke, myocardial infarction, symptomatic angina pectoris, transient cerebral ischemia, or revascularization procedures, such as percutaneous transluminal angioplasty, coronary artery bypass graft, carotid endarterectomy, or peripheral artery surgery). In April 1998, a fourth study arm for B vitamin supplementation was added among 5442 women.15 Until July 2005, participants completed annual questionnaires on compliance, adverse effects, health and lifestyle, and clinical end points. None of the supplements used were associated with CVD recurrence14,15 or with cognitive change.16,17

COGNITIVE SUBCOHORT

Between July 1998 and December 2000, we assessed cognitive function by telephone interview among participants 65 years or older. Of 3170 eligible women, 190 were unreachable, 156 declined participation, and 2824 (94.8% of 2980 contacted women) completed the initial assessment. Participants received 3 follow-up assessments at 2-year intervals until July 2005; 92.5% completed at least 1 follow-up assessment, and 81.0% completed at least 3 assessments. For the fourth assessment, 24.3% were not contacted, as only a short interval had passed between their third interview and the end of the trial. We excluded 15 participants with prevalent Parkinson disease who likely had cognitive impairment and did not engage in regular physical activity. Therefore, the analyses included 2809 women. This study was approved by the institutional review board of Brigham and Women's Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts.

PHYSICAL ACTIVITY ASSESSMENT

At baseline and biennially thereafter, women were asked about their mean weekly time spent during the past year on the following: walking or hiking; lap swimming; tennis, squash, or racquetball; jogging (speed <10-minute miles); running (speed ≥10-minute miles); bicycling, including the use of stationary machines; aerobic exercise, aerobic dance, or the use of exercise machines; and lower-intensity exercise, including yoga, stretching, or toning. We also inquired about the number of flights of stairs climbed daily (0, 1-2, 3-4, 5-9, 10-14, or ≥15) and the usual pace of walking (<3.2 km/h [<2.0 mph, easy pace], 3.2-4.7 km/h [2.0-2.9 mph, normal pace], 4.8-6.3 km/h [3.0-3.9 mph, brisk pace], or ≥6.4 km/h [≥4.0 mph, very brisk pace]). We assigned each physical activity a metabolic equivalent of task (MET) value, where 1 MET is proportional to the energy expended while sitting quietly.18 MET values were 12 for running; 8 for stair-climbing; 7 for jogging, racquet sports, lap swimming, and bicycling; 6 for aerobic exercise, dance, or the use of exercise machines; and 4 for yoga, stretching, or toning. MET values for walking varied by pace, from 2.5 METs for an easy pace to 4.5 METs for a very brisk pace. For each physical activity, we estimated the energy expended in MET hours per week by multiplying its MET value by the duration.

In a validation study among comparable female participants in a large cohort study,19 the physical activity responses given 2 years apart were reasonably correlated (r = 0.6) given the expected true changes that might occur. Moreover, the physical activity recalled for the previous year correlated with activity based on past-week recalls (r = 0.8) and activity diaries during the year (r = 0.6).

COGNITIVE ASSESSMENT

Cognitive function was assessed by telephone interview using 5 tests. Global cognition was evaluated using the Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (TICS),20 a telephone adaptation of the Mini-Mental State Examination (score range, 0-41 points). Verbal memory was assessed using the TICS 10-word list (immediate and delayed recalls) and the East Boston Memory Test (immediate and delayed recalls).21 Women were asked to name as many animals as possible in 1 minute in a test of category fluency.22

Our primary outcome was the rate of change in the global cognition composite score, computed as the mean of the z scores from all cognitive tests. As secondary outcomes, we considered the changes in TICS score, verbal memory composite score (mean of the verbal memory z scores), and category fluency score. Verbal memory is one of the best predictors of Alzheimer disease,23 and category fluency partly measures executive function, which is associated with vascular dementia.24,25 To derive the composite scores for 0.5% of participants who did not complete all tests, we used the means of the z scores from the available relevant tests.

In a validation study among 61 women, the global cognition composite score from the brief telephone-administered assessment correlated strongly with the total score from an extensive in-person interview for cognition (r = 0.8). In a reliability study among 35 high-functioning educated women, the correlation between TICS scores administered twice 31 days apart was 0.7. Among 88 older female health professionals who were demographically similar to Women's Antioxidant Cardiovascular Study participants, poor performances on the TICS and the verbal memory composite score were associated with significant 8- and 12-fold increases, respectively, in subsequent dementia diagnoses.6 Therefore, extensive evidence supports the validity of our telephone cognitive instrument.

COVARIATES

We considered several potential confounding factors plausibly linked with both physical activity and cognitive decline. Basic models included education and age at the initial cognitive assessment. Multivariable models further included the Women's Antioxidant Cardiovascular Study randomization assignments, as well as numerous lifestyle and health variables listed in Table 1, with covariate specifications given in a footnote of Table 2.

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 1. Baseline Characteristics by Quintile of Total Physical Activity Among 2809 Women in the Women's Antioxidant Cardiovascular Study Cognitive Cohort
Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 2. Adjusted Differences in Annual Rates of Cognitive Change for Various Cognitive Scores Over 4 Assessments by Quintile of Total Physical Activity at Baseline Among 2809 Women in the Women's Antioxidant Cardiovascular Study Cognitive Cohort
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Physical Activity

We examined quintiles of total energy expended in all activities and quartiles of energy expended in walking (walking had a narrower distribution of energy expenditure), which was the most common physical activity. Before the first cognitive assessment, physical activity was assessed at randomization and again after 24 months. To reduce the effect of any recent changes in physical activity because of health or cognitive status (ie, reverse causation bias), our main analyses were based on the energy expenditures from the baseline questionnaire, which were assessed a mean of 3.5 years before the first cognitive assessment. However, given our interest in longer-term consistent physical activity levels, in secondary analyses we also examined the mean energy expenditures from reports on baseline and 24-month questionnaires. Furthermore, we examined associations among a subset of women whose physical activity remained stable (ie, in the same or adjacent quintile at baseline and at 24 months).

When examining walking for exercise, we controlled for the energy levels expended in other activities as potential confounders. To isolate the effects of walking, we also conducted a stratified analysis among 1387 women who only reported walking or engaging in low-intensity exercises for their activities.

Statistical Models

We used general linear models for repeated measures with random intercepts and slopes with an unstructured covariance matrix to estimate the association of physical activity with the annual rate of cognitive change. We assessed linear trends across physical activity categories by testing a continuous variable in which participants were assigned the median of the category. We used the Wald test for statistical testing. Models were fitted by the maximum likelihood method using commercially available software (SAS, version 9.1; SAS Institute, Inc, Cary, North Carolina).

Because age, education, depression, diabetes, hypertension, or cardiovascular profile could modify the association between physical activity and cognitive decline, we evaluated 3-way interaction terms of time, physical activity level, and the potential effect modifier in separate multivariable-adjusted models. We performed an analysis that excluded women who had the worst cognitive function at the initial assessment (defined as the worst 10% of the distribution) to reduce any potential bias owing to less healthy behaviors or worse reporting in this group.

The mean time from the first physical activity assessment to the initial cognitive assessment was 3.5 years (range, 3.1-4.7 years), and the mean time from the initial cognitive assessment to the last cognitive assessment was 5.4 years (range, 4.0-6.1 years). Participants who were lost to follow-up tended to be older and less active at baseline.

Walking accounted for about half of the total energy expenditure (Table 1). Women with higher physical activity had lower mean body mass index, were more likely to consume alcohol, and were less likely to smoke or report lung disease, diabetes, or hypertension. Physical activity was not associated with CVD.

TOTAL PHYSICAL ACTIVITY

More active women tended to have better cognitive scores over time (Figure), and the difference in performance by physical activity level widened over time. In the basic and multivariable-adjusted models, greater physical activity was significantly associated with slower declines in the global cognition score (P < .001 for trend), TICS (P = .03 for trend), and verbal memory score (P < .001 for trend) but was not associated with the category fluency score (Table 2). We found statistically significant differences in global cognitive decline beginning with women in the fourth (P = .04) and fifth (P < .001) quintiles of total energy expenditure, which was equivalent to walking 30 minutes or more every day at a brisk pace (ie, 5.5 km/h [3.5 mph]). Because the mean differences in cognitive decline can be difficult to interpret, we compared the estimates for physical activity and cognitive decline with those for age and cognitive decline; therefore, the effect of age on cognitive decline was used as a benchmark for interpretation. The mean difference in rates of cognitive decline between the first and fourth quintiles of physical activity was equivalent to the mean difference found for women 5 years apart in age, and the mean difference between the first and fifth quintiles was equivalent to 7 years of age. That is, the apparent cognitive benefits associated with physical activity levels, such as walking 30 minutes or longer every day at a brisk pace, were equivalent to being cognitively younger by 5 to 7 years.

Place holder to copy figure label and caption
Graphic Jump Location

Figure. Adjusted global cognition composite score (mean of z scores) during the cognitive follow-up period (1998-2005) by quintile of total physical activity at baseline (n = 2809). Trend curves are shown for quintiles 1, 3, and 5. For the fourth assessment, 24.3% of participants were not contacted, as only a short interval had passed between their third interview and the parent trial end; therefore, there is more statistical variation for this assessment.

In alternate analyses, when we considered the mean of the 2 physical activity measures before the first cognitive evaluation, the associations were similar and somewhat stronger, notably for the category fluency test: comparing the first quintile with the fifth quintile, the difference in annual cognitive decline was 0.03 standard units (95% confidence interval [CI], 0.01-0.05 standard units) for the global cognition score (P < .001 for trend), 0.12 points (95% CI, 0.03-0.22 points) for the TICS score (P = .003 for trend), 0.03 standard units (95% CI, 0.01-0.05 standard units) for the verbal memory score (P < .001 for trend), and 0.12 points (95% CI, −0.01 to 0.25 points) for the category fluency score (P = .09 for trend). In addition, among the subset of 1971 women with stable physical activity levels on the 2 assessments before cognitive evaluations, results were also similar: comparing the first quintile with fifth quintile, the difference in annual cognitive decline was 0.04 standard units (95% CI, 0.01-0.06 standard units) for the global cognition score (P < .001 for trend), 0.13 points (95% CI, 0.02-0.25 points) for the TICS score (P = .002 for trend), 0.04 standard units (95% CI, 0.01-0.06 standard units) for the verbal memory score (P < .001 for trend), and 0.14 points (95% CI, −0.02 to 0.30 points) for the category fluency score (P = .17 for trend).

WALKING

In models adjusted for other physical activities, we found significantly slower rates of decline in the global cognition, verbal memory, and category fluency scores associated with increased walking for exercise (P = .003, P = .01, and P = .03 for trend, respectively) (Table 3). However, significant associations were observed with the last quartile only (with a minimum of daily 30-minute walks), indicating a possible threshold effect. The mean difference in rates of cognitive decline between the first and fourth quartiles of walking was equivalent to the mean difference found for women 5 years apart in age.

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 3. Adjusted Differences in Annual Rates of Cognitive Change for Various Cognitive Scores Over 4 Assessments by Quartile of Walking at Baseline Among 2809 Women in the Women's Antioxidant Cardiovascular Study Cognitive Cohort

Furthermore, in the restricted group of 1387 women who did not engage in any vigorous physical activity, the associations with walking remained similar, although the results were of borderline significance. For example, the difference in the mean decline for the global cognition score was 0.02 standard units (95% CI, 0.00-0.05 standard units) (P = .08 for trend) when contrasting the lowest quartile with the highest quartile of walking.

EFFECT MODIFICATION AND FURTHER STRATIFIED ANALYSES

We observed no significant interactions with the variables we tested as potential effect modifiers (ie, age, education, and depression, diabetes, and hypertension and cardiovascular profiles). Models limited to women in the top 90th percentile of global cognition composite score at the first cognitive interview (ie, women with preserved cognitive function) yielded similar results as the primary analyses, with P = .004 for trend across quintiles of total physical activity for global cognition and verbal memory scores.

In this large prospective study of women with preexisting CVD or vascular risk factors at high risk of cognitive decline, greater physical activity was associated with substantially slower cognitive decline. Participating in the 2 highest quintiles of physical activity was cognitively equivalent to being 5 to 7 years younger. Most important, the association with total physical activity was not restricted to women engaged in vigorous exercise; higher levels of walking for exercise were significantly related to less cognitive decline.

Strong evidence supports the hypothesis that physical activity, including walking, may prevent cognitive decline in generally healthy older adults.412 However, studies of those at increased risk of cognitive impairment have been scarce. Small clinical trials among participants with general cognitive impairments9,11 or with severe congestive heart failure26 found improvement or preservation of cognition in the physically active groups, but overall the definitions of high risk have been heterogeneous (eg, general cognitive problems rather than specific causes of cognitive impairments), yielding inconsistent and inconclusive results.5,7,27,28 Therefore, our findings provide important population-based long-term data that should be confirmed.

In our study, associations with change in category fluency score were less consistent than those for global cognition and verbal memory scores; this was also observed in a cohort of healthy women.6 Category fluency score partially measures executive function, which is known to be affected by vascular disease.24,25 One could speculate that the indirect vasculoprotective effects are weaker than the direct neuroprotective effects in preserving cognition and that the domain most affected by vascular factors, such as executive function, may be less influenced than other domains. The potential differential associations with various cognitive domains are poorly understood. In addition, short-term intervention studies11,2931 and large observational cohorts4,6,10,12 have revealed the possibility of differential effects with sex and various types of activities. These are important issues that need evaluation in future studies.

Our study had several strengths. Four successive cognitive assessments with high response rates were completed, maximizing information and minimizing biases because of loss to follow-up. Extensive health-related information was available, which allowed us to address confounding by baseline health status. Moreover, physical activity levels showed little variability according to baseline vascular disease, indicating low chance of confounding by severity of cardiovascular condition.

Some limitations should be considered. First, a telephone cognitive assessment might lack validity. However, reliability and validity studies of our telephone instrument provided convincing evidence of its usefulness to evaluate cognitive function in an epidemiologic study. Second, there may have been some misclassification of physical activity levels that were based on self-report. However, the physical activity questionnaire has been shown to reliably estimate physical activity levels,19 and misclassification would have led to biases toward the null. Third and most important, physical activity was assessed in late adulthood and may not reflect long-term exercise levels; it is also possible that inactivity or a sedentary lifestyle may represent preexisting cognitive impairment rather than a risk factor for its future development.32 We addressed this possible bias in several ways; we imposed a mean 3.5-year lag between report of physical activity and the initial cognitive assessment, and we conducted several secondary analyses among women whose activity levels were stable over the 2 assessments before the cognitive assessment and among women in the top 90th percentile at the first cognitive interview (ie, after excluding those more likely to have reduced physical activity or have errors in reporting activity because of cognitive impairment). Overall, we confirmed that higher levels of physical activity were consistently and significantly associated with less cognitive decline. We were unable to adjust for other potential confounders (eg, other psychiatric disorders or chronic kidney disease); therefore, some residual confounding by additional health or lifestyle factors is a possibility, and the results should be interpreted with appropriate caution. Fourth, our study population, which was composed of female health professionals with vascular conditions, may not allow for direct generalizability of the results to the general aging population. However, given that most of today's older population has prevalent CVD (affecting >70% of those aged ≥65 years in the United States3), our study provides important evidence (which should be confirmed in other studies) of a modifiable risk factor for reducing cognitive impairment in this growing segment of the population.

Various biologic mechanisms may explain the positive relation between physical activity and cognitive health.33 Exercise may directly preserve neuronal structures by stimulating brain-derived neurotrophic factor and neuronal growth,34 possibly providing reserve against cognitive decline and dementia.35 Exercise may also have indirect effects by strengthening the underlying systems that support brain plasticity36,37 and helping to sustain the brain's vascular health38 by beneficially influencing cardiovascular risk factors, promoting endothelial function, improving glucose and insulin regulation, and ensuring adequate cerebral perfusion.39 Furthermore, physical activity reduces inflammation, which is higher in those with vascular disease40,41 and impairs systemic and brain-specific growth factor signaling. Physical activity may also improve psychological well-being,42 which in turn may protect against decline in cognitive functioning.43

In summary, we found clear and strong associations between greater physical activity and reduced cognitive decline in this population of women with vascular disease or coronary risk factors. If confirmed in future studies, physical activity recommendations could yield substantial public health benefits given the growing number of older persons with vascular conditions and their high risk of cognitive impairment.

Correspondence: Jae H. Kang, ScD, Channing Laboratory and Division of Preventive Medicine, Department of Medicine, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, 181 Longwood Ave, Boston, MA 02115 (nhjhk@channing.harvard.edu).

Accepted for Publication: April 25, 2011.

Published Online: July 19, 2011. doi:10.1001/archinternmed.2011.282

Author Contributions: Dr Kang had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: Vercambre, Grodstein, and Kang. Acquisition of data: Grodstein, Manson, and Kang. Analysis and interpretation of data: Vercambre, Grodstein, Manson, Stampfer, and Kang. Drafting of the manuscript: Vercambre. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Grodstein, Manson, Stampfer, and Kang. Statistical analysis: Vercambre. Obtained funding: Grodstein and Kang. Study supervision: Kang.

Financial Disclosure: None reported.

Funding/Support: This work was supported by grants HL046959 and AG15933 from the National Institutes of Health. Dr Vercambre's postdoctoral fellowship is supported by the Fondation Bettencourt-Schueller. Dr Kang is the recipient of a Scientist Development Award from the American Heart Association.

Role of the Sponsors: The American Heart Association financially supported the study but was not involved in any of the following: design or conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, or interpretation of the data; or preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.

Previous Presentation: An abstract of this work was presented at the Alzheimer's Association 2011 International Conference on Alzheimer's Disease; July 19, 2011; Paris, France.

Additional Contributions: We thank the investigators, staff, and participants of the Women's Antioxidant Cardiovascular Study cognitive substudy.

de la Torre JC. How do heart disease and stroke become risk factors for Alzheimer's disease?  Neurol Res. 2006;28(6):637-644
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Reitz C, Tang MX, Schupf N, Manly JJ, Mayeux R, Luchsinger JA. A summary risk score for the prediction of Alzheimer disease in elderly persons.  Arch Neurol. 2010;67(7):835-841
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Lloyd-Jones D, Adams R, Carnethon M,  et al; American Heart Association Statistics Committee and Stroke Statistics Subcommittee.  Heart disease and stroke statistics: 2009 update: a report from the American Heart Association Statistics Committee and Stroke Statistics Subcommittee [published corrections appear in Circulation. 2010;122(1):e11 and 2009;119(3):e182].  Circulation. 2009;119(3):e21-e181.http://circ.ahajournals.org/cgi/content/full/119/3/e21. Accessed May 9, 2011
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Yaffe K, Barnes D, Nevitt M, Lui LY, Covinsky K. A prospective study of physical activity and cognitive decline in elderly women: women who walk.  Arch Intern Med. 2001;161(14):1703-1708
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Schuit AJ, Feskens EJ, Launer LJ, Kromhout D. Physical activity and cognitive decline, the role of the apolipoprotein e4 allele.  Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2001;33(5):772-777
PubMed
Weuve J, Kang JH, Manson JE, Breteler MM, Ware JH, Grodstein F. Physical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older women.  JAMA. 2004;292(12):1454-1461
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Podewils LJ, Guallar E, Kuller LH,  et al.  Physical activity, APOE genotype, and dementia risk: findings from the Cardiovascular Health Cognition Study.  Am J Epidemiol. 2005;161(7):639-651
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Cassilhas RC, Viana VA, Grassmann V,  et al.  The impact of resistance exercise on the cognitive function of the elderly.  Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39(8):1401-1407
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Lautenschlager NT, Cox KL, Flicker L,  et al.  Effect of physical activity on cognitive function in older adults at risk for Alzheimer disease: a randomized trial.  JAMA. 2008;300(9):1027-1037
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Middleton LE, Mitnitski A, Fallah N, Kirkland SA, Rockwood K. Changes in cognition and mortality in relation to exercise in late life: a population based study.  PLoS One. 2008;3(9):e3124http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2518854/?tool=pubmed. Accessed May 9, 2011
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Baker LD, Frank LL, Foster-Schubert K,  et al.  Effects of aerobic exercise on mild cognitive impairment: a controlled trial.  Arch Neurol. 2010;67(1):71-79
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Etgen T, Sander D, Huntgeburth U, Poppert H, Förstl H, Bickel H. Physical activity and incident cognitive impairment in elderly persons: the INVADE study.  Arch Intern Med. 2010;170(2):186-193
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Jolliffe JA, Rees K, Taylor RS, Thompson D, Oldridge N, Ebrahim S. Exercise-based rehabilitation for coronary heart disease.  Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2000;(4):CD001800
PubMed
Cook NR, Albert CM, Gaziano JM,  et al.  A randomized factorial trial of vitamins C and E and β-carotene in the secondary prevention of cardiovascular events in women: results from the Women's Antioxidant Cardiovascular Study.  Arch Intern Med. 2007;167(15):1610-1618
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Albert CM, Cook NR, Gaziano JM,  et al.  Effect of folic acid and B vitamins on risk of cardiovascular events and total mortality among women at high risk for cardiovascular disease: a randomized trial.  JAMA. 2008;299(17):2027-2036
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Kang JH, Cook N, Manson J, Buring JE, Albert CM, Grodstein F. A trial of B vitamins and cognitive function among women at high risk of cardiovascular disease.  Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;88(6):1602-1610
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Kang JH, Cook NR, Manson JE, Buring JE, Albert CM, Grodstein F. Vitamin E, vitamin C, β-carotene, and cognitive function among women with or at risk of cardiovascular disease: the Women's Antioxidant and Cardiovascular Study.  Circulation. 2009;119(21):2772-2780
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Ainsworth BE, Haskell WL, Leon AS,  et al.  Compendium of physical activities: classification of energy costs of human physical activities.  Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1993;25(1):71-80
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Wolf AM, Hunter DJ, Colditz GA,  et al.  Reproducibility and validity of a self-administered physical activity questionnaire.  Int J Epidemiol. 1994;23(5):991-999
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Brandt J, Folstein MF. Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status: Professional Manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc; 2003
Scherr PA, Albert MS, Funkenstein HH,  et al.  Correlates of cognitive function in an elderly community population.  Am J Epidemiol. 1988;128(5):1084-1101
PubMed
Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC,  et al.  The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD), part I: clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer's disease.  Neurology. 1989;39(9):1159-1165
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Small BJ, Fratiglioni L, Viitanen M, Winblad B, Bäckman L. The course of cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer disease: three- and 6-year follow-up of a population-based sample.  Arch Neurol. 2000;57(6):839-844
PubMed   |  Link to Article
O’Brien JT. Vascular cognitive impairment.  Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14(9):724-733
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Desmond DW. The neuropsychology of vascular cognitive impairment: is there a specific cognitive deficit?  J Neurol Sci. 2004;226(1-2):3-7
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Tanne D, Freimark D, Poreh A,  et al.  Cognitive functions in severe congestive heart failure before and after an exercise training program.  Int J Cardiol. 2005;103(2):145-149
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Ploughman M, McCarthy J, Bossé M, Sullivan HJ, Corbett D. Does treadmill exercise improve performance of cognitive or upper-extremity tasks in people with chronic stroke? a randomized cross-over trial.  Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2008;89(11):2041-2047
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Devore EE, Kang JH, Okereke O, Grodstein F. Physical activity levels and cognition in women with type 2 diabetes.  Am J Epidemiol. 2009;170(8):1040-1047
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Angevaren M, Aufdemkampe G, Verhaar HJ, Aleman A, Vanhees L. Physical activity and enhanced fitness to improve cognitive function in older people without known cognitive impairment.  Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2008;(3):CD005381
PubMed
Liu-Ambrose T, Nagamatsu LS, Graf P, Beattie BL, Ashe MC, Handy TC. Resistance training and executive functions: a 12-month randomized controlled trial.  Arch Intern Med. 2010;170(2):170-178
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Klusmann V, Evers A, Schwarzer R,  et al.  Complex mental and physical activity in older women and cognitive performance: a 6-month randomized controlled trial.  J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2010;65(6):680-688
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Solfrizzi V, Capurso C, D’Introno A,  et al.  Lifestyle-related factors in predementia and dementia syndromes.  Expert Rev Neurother. 2008;8(1):133-158
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Cotman CW, Berchtold NC, Christie LA. Exercise builds brain health: key roles of growth factor cascades and inflammation.  Trends Neurosci. 2007;30(9):464-472
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Kramer AF, Erickson KI. Capitalizing on cortical plasticity: influence of physical activity on cognition and brain function.  Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11(8):342-348
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Fratiglioni L, Paillard-Borg S, Winblad B. An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia.  Lancet Neurol. 2004;3(6):343-353
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Burns JM, Cronk BB, Anderson HS,  et al.  Cardiorespiratory fitness and brain atrophy in early Alzheimer disease.  Neurology. 2008;71(3):210-216
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS,  et al.  Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(7):3017-3022
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Thompson PD, Buchner D, Pina IL,  et al; American Heart Association Council on Clinical Cardiology Subcommittee on Exercise, Rehabilitation, and Prevention; American Heart Association Council on Nutrition, Physical Activity, and Metabolism Subcommittee on Physical Activity.  Exercise and physical activity in the prevention and treatment of atherosclerotic cardiovascular disease: a statement from the Council on Clinical Cardiology (Subcommittee on Exercise, Rehabilitation, and Prevention) and the Council on Nutrition, Physical Activity, and Metabolism (Subcommittee on Physical Activity).  Circulation. 2003;107(24):3109-3116
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Rosano C, Newman AB. Cardiovascular disease and risk of Alzheimer's disease.  Neurol Res. 2006;28(6):612-620
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Hansson GK, Robertson AK, Söderberg-Nauclér C. Inflammation and atherosclerosis.  Annu Rev Pathol. 2006;1:297-329
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Hamer M, Stamatakis E. Physical activity and risk of cardiovascular disease events: inflammatory and metabolic mechanisms.  Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2009;41(6):1206-1211
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Vance DE, Wadley VG, Ball KK, Roenker DL, Rizzo M. The effects of physical activity and sedentary behavior on cognitive health in older adults.  J Aging Phys Act. 2005;13(3):294-313
PubMed
Beaudreau SA, O’Hara R. The association of anxiety and depressive symptoms with cognitive performance in community-dwelling older adults.  Psychol Aging. 2009;24(2):507-512
PubMed   |  Link to Article

Figures

Place holder to copy figure label and caption
Graphic Jump Location

Figure. Adjusted global cognition composite score (mean of z scores) during the cognitive follow-up period (1998-2005) by quintile of total physical activity at baseline (n = 2809). Trend curves are shown for quintiles 1, 3, and 5. For the fourth assessment, 24.3% of participants were not contacted, as only a short interval had passed between their third interview and the parent trial end; therefore, there is more statistical variation for this assessment.

Tables

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 1. Baseline Characteristics by Quintile of Total Physical Activity Among 2809 Women in the Women's Antioxidant Cardiovascular Study Cognitive Cohort
Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 2. Adjusted Differences in Annual Rates of Cognitive Change for Various Cognitive Scores Over 4 Assessments by Quintile of Total Physical Activity at Baseline Among 2809 Women in the Women's Antioxidant Cardiovascular Study Cognitive Cohort
Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 3. Adjusted Differences in Annual Rates of Cognitive Change for Various Cognitive Scores Over 4 Assessments by Quartile of Walking at Baseline Among 2809 Women in the Women's Antioxidant Cardiovascular Study Cognitive Cohort

References

de la Torre JC. How do heart disease and stroke become risk factors for Alzheimer's disease?  Neurol Res. 2006;28(6):637-644
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Reitz C, Tang MX, Schupf N, Manly JJ, Mayeux R, Luchsinger JA. A summary risk score for the prediction of Alzheimer disease in elderly persons.  Arch Neurol. 2010;67(7):835-841
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Lloyd-Jones D, Adams R, Carnethon M,  et al; American Heart Association Statistics Committee and Stroke Statistics Subcommittee.  Heart disease and stroke statistics: 2009 update: a report from the American Heart Association Statistics Committee and Stroke Statistics Subcommittee [published corrections appear in Circulation. 2010;122(1):e11 and 2009;119(3):e182].  Circulation. 2009;119(3):e21-e181.http://circ.ahajournals.org/cgi/content/full/119/3/e21. Accessed May 9, 2011
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Yaffe K, Barnes D, Nevitt M, Lui LY, Covinsky K. A prospective study of physical activity and cognitive decline in elderly women: women who walk.  Arch Intern Med. 2001;161(14):1703-1708
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Schuit AJ, Feskens EJ, Launer LJ, Kromhout D. Physical activity and cognitive decline, the role of the apolipoprotein e4 allele.  Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2001;33(5):772-777
PubMed
Weuve J, Kang JH, Manson JE, Breteler MM, Ware JH, Grodstein F. Physical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older women.  JAMA. 2004;292(12):1454-1461
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Podewils LJ, Guallar E, Kuller LH,  et al.  Physical activity, APOE genotype, and dementia risk: findings from the Cardiovascular Health Cognition Study.  Am J Epidemiol. 2005;161(7):639-651
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Cassilhas RC, Viana VA, Grassmann V,  et al.  The impact of resistance exercise on the cognitive function of the elderly.  Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39(8):1401-1407
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Lautenschlager NT, Cox KL, Flicker L,  et al.  Effect of physical activity on cognitive function in older adults at risk for Alzheimer disease: a randomized trial.  JAMA. 2008;300(9):1027-1037
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Middleton LE, Mitnitski A, Fallah N, Kirkland SA, Rockwood K. Changes in cognition and mortality in relation to exercise in late life: a population based study.  PLoS One. 2008;3(9):e3124http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2518854/?tool=pubmed. Accessed May 9, 2011
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Baker LD, Frank LL, Foster-Schubert K,  et al.  Effects of aerobic exercise on mild cognitive impairment: a controlled trial.  Arch Neurol. 2010;67(1):71-79
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Etgen T, Sander D, Huntgeburth U, Poppert H, Förstl H, Bickel H. Physical activity and incident cognitive impairment in elderly persons: the INVADE study.  Arch Intern Med. 2010;170(2):186-193
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Jolliffe JA, Rees K, Taylor RS, Thompson D, Oldridge N, Ebrahim S. Exercise-based rehabilitation for coronary heart disease.  Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2000;(4):CD001800
PubMed
Cook NR, Albert CM, Gaziano JM,  et al.  A randomized factorial trial of vitamins C and E and β-carotene in the secondary prevention of cardiovascular events in women: results from the Women's Antioxidant Cardiovascular Study.  Arch Intern Med. 2007;167(15):1610-1618
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Albert CM, Cook NR, Gaziano JM,  et al.  Effect of folic acid and B vitamins on risk of cardiovascular events and total mortality among women at high risk for cardiovascular disease: a randomized trial.  JAMA. 2008;299(17):2027-2036
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Kang JH, Cook N, Manson J, Buring JE, Albert CM, Grodstein F. A trial of B vitamins and cognitive function among women at high risk of cardiovascular disease.  Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;88(6):1602-1610
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Kang JH, Cook NR, Manson JE, Buring JE, Albert CM, Grodstein F. Vitamin E, vitamin C, β-carotene, and cognitive function among women with or at risk of cardiovascular disease: the Women's Antioxidant and Cardiovascular Study.  Circulation. 2009;119(21):2772-2780
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Ainsworth BE, Haskell WL, Leon AS,  et al.  Compendium of physical activities: classification of energy costs of human physical activities.  Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1993;25(1):71-80
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Wolf AM, Hunter DJ, Colditz GA,  et al.  Reproducibility and validity of a self-administered physical activity questionnaire.  Int J Epidemiol. 1994;23(5):991-999
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Brandt J, Folstein MF. Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status: Professional Manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc; 2003
Scherr PA, Albert MS, Funkenstein HH,  et al.  Correlates of cognitive function in an elderly community population.  Am J Epidemiol. 1988;128(5):1084-1101
PubMed
Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC,  et al.  The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD), part I: clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer's disease.  Neurology. 1989;39(9):1159-1165
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Small BJ, Fratiglioni L, Viitanen M, Winblad B, Bäckman L. The course of cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer disease: three- and 6-year follow-up of a population-based sample.  Arch Neurol. 2000;57(6):839-844
PubMed   |  Link to Article
O’Brien JT. Vascular cognitive impairment.  Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14(9):724-733
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Desmond DW. The neuropsychology of vascular cognitive impairment: is there a specific cognitive deficit?  J Neurol Sci. 2004;226(1-2):3-7
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Tanne D, Freimark D, Poreh A,  et al.  Cognitive functions in severe congestive heart failure before and after an exercise training program.  Int J Cardiol. 2005;103(2):145-149
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Ploughman M, McCarthy J, Bossé M, Sullivan HJ, Corbett D. Does treadmill exercise improve performance of cognitive or upper-extremity tasks in people with chronic stroke? a randomized cross-over trial.  Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2008;89(11):2041-2047
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Devore EE, Kang JH, Okereke O, Grodstein F. Physical activity levels and cognition in women with type 2 diabetes.  Am J Epidemiol. 2009;170(8):1040-1047
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Angevaren M, Aufdemkampe G, Verhaar HJ, Aleman A, Vanhees L. Physical activity and enhanced fitness to improve cognitive function in older people without known cognitive impairment.  Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2008;(3):CD005381
PubMed
Liu-Ambrose T, Nagamatsu LS, Graf P, Beattie BL, Ashe MC, Handy TC. Resistance training and executive functions: a 12-month randomized controlled trial.  Arch Intern Med. 2010;170(2):170-178
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Klusmann V, Evers A, Schwarzer R,  et al.  Complex mental and physical activity in older women and cognitive performance: a 6-month randomized controlled trial.  J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2010;65(6):680-688
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Solfrizzi V, Capurso C, D’Introno A,  et al.  Lifestyle-related factors in predementia and dementia syndromes.  Expert Rev Neurother. 2008;8(1):133-158
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Cotman CW, Berchtold NC, Christie LA. Exercise builds brain health: key roles of growth factor cascades and inflammation.  Trends Neurosci. 2007;30(9):464-472
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Kramer AF, Erickson KI. Capitalizing on cortical plasticity: influence of physical activity on cognition and brain function.  Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11(8):342-348
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Fratiglioni L, Paillard-Borg S, Winblad B. An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia.  Lancet Neurol. 2004;3(6):343-353
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Burns JM, Cronk BB, Anderson HS,  et al.  Cardiorespiratory fitness and brain atrophy in early Alzheimer disease.  Neurology. 2008;71(3):210-216
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS,  et al.  Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(7):3017-3022
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Thompson PD, Buchner D, Pina IL,  et al; American Heart Association Council on Clinical Cardiology Subcommittee on Exercise, Rehabilitation, and Prevention; American Heart Association Council on Nutrition, Physical Activity, and Metabolism Subcommittee on Physical Activity.  Exercise and physical activity in the prevention and treatment of atherosclerotic cardiovascular disease: a statement from the Council on Clinical Cardiology (Subcommittee on Exercise, Rehabilitation, and Prevention) and the Council on Nutrition, Physical Activity, and Metabolism (Subcommittee on Physical Activity).  Circulation. 2003;107(24):3109-3116
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Rosano C, Newman AB. Cardiovascular disease and risk of Alzheimer's disease.  Neurol Res. 2006;28(6):612-620
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Hansson GK, Robertson AK, Söderberg-Nauclér C. Inflammation and atherosclerosis.  Annu Rev Pathol. 2006;1:297-329
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Hamer M, Stamatakis E. Physical activity and risk of cardiovascular disease events: inflammatory and metabolic mechanisms.  Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2009;41(6):1206-1211
PubMed   |  Link to Article
Vance DE, Wadley VG, Ball KK, Roenker DL, Rizzo M. The effects of physical activity and sedentary behavior on cognitive health in older adults.  J Aging Phys Act. 2005;13(3):294-313
PubMed
Beaudreau SA, O’Hara R. The association of anxiety and depressive symptoms with cognitive performance in community-dwelling older adults.  Psychol Aging. 2009;24(2):507-512
PubMed   |  Link to Article

Correspondence

CME
Meets CME requirements for:
Browse CME for all U.S. States
Accreditation Information
The American Medical Association is accredited by the Accreditation Council for Continuing Medical Education to provide continuing medical education for physicians. The AMA designates this journal-based CME activity for a maximum of 1 AMA PRA Category 1 CreditTM per course. Physicians should claim only the credit commensurate with the extent of their participation in the activity. Physicians who complete the CME course and score at least 80% correct on the quiz are eligible for AMA PRA Category 1 CreditTM.
Note: You must get at least of the answers correct to pass this quiz.
You have not filled in all the answers to complete this quiz
The following questions were not answered:
Sorry, you have unsuccessfully completed this CME quiz with a score of
The following questions were not answered correctly:
Commitment to Change (optional):
Indicate what change(s) you will implement in your practice, if any, based on this CME course.
Your quiz results:
The filled radio buttons indicate your responses. The preferred responses are highlighted
For CME Course: A Proposed Model for Initial Assessment and Management of Acute Heart Failure Syndromes
Indicate what changes(s) you will implement in your practice, if any, based on this CME course.
Submit a Comment

Multimedia

Some tools below are only available to our subscribers or users with an online account.

Web of Science® Times Cited: 18

Related Content

Customize your page view by dragging & repositioning the boxes below.

See Also...
Articles Related By Topic
Related Collections
PubMed Articles